为了正常的体验网站,请在浏览器设置里面开启Javascript功能!

被拒绝者更容易辨别真假微笑

2012-03-02 4页 pdf 228KB 17阅读

用户头像

is_253930

暂无简介

举报
被拒绝者更容易辨别真假微笑 g . ject (no t re ver xclu enn ork vidu as opposed to ‘‘fake” smiles. This effect was partially mediated by threats to ‘‘relational needs” (Williams, responses that could facilitate reconnection with others. Published by Elsevier Inc. ial, and ersive hat hu cti...
被拒绝者更容易辨别真假微笑
g . ject (no t re ver xclu enn ork vidu as opposed to ‘‘fake” smiles. This effect was partially mediated by threats to ‘‘relational needs” (Williams, responses that could facilitate reconnection with others. Published by Elsevier Inc. ial, and ersive hat hu cting d (e.g., L duals greater performer control, can conceal negative emotions or fake the desired positivity associated with a real smile (Ekman, Friesen, & O’Sullivan, 1988). Unlike non-Duchenne smiles, Duchenne smiles are strong signals of the person’s cooperative intent (Brown & Moore, 2002); people tend to exhibit more Duchenne smiles while non-Duchenne smiles, it did not showwhether rejected individuals ‘‘use” this information in any way. The identification of such smiles is only beneficial if such discrimination produces responses useful for satiating current needs. Thus, excluded individuals should also prefer to interact with persons expressing true approach displays (e.g., Duchenne smiles) rather than those not expressing such dis- plays, which is an untested hypothesis. Moreover, previous researchers explicitly directed participants’ attention to the verac- ity of the smiles. It remains unclear whether rejected individuals * Corresponding author. Address: Department of Psychology, Psychology Build- ing, Miami University, Oxford, OH 45056, United States. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46 (2010) 196–199 Contents lists availab t .e E-mail address: bernstmj@muohio.edu (M.J. Bernstein). their need to belong better identify social information, like facial expressions (Pickett, Gardner, & Knowles, 2004), and those who experience exclusion engage in greater behavioral mimicry (Lakin, Chartrand, & Arkin, 2008), an indication that individuals fearing or experiencing rejection show increased attention to social cues use- ful for securing reaffiliation. One rather powerful cue of affiliation intent is positive affect, which is often communicated by smiling (e.g., Brown, Palameta, & Moore, 2003). Specifically, ‘‘Duchenne” (genuine) smiles occur automatically in response to the experience of happiness (Ekman, Davidson, & Friesen, 1990). Non-Duchenne smiles, which are under previously found that those recalling an exclusion experience could better discriminate between Duchenne and non-Duchenne smiles than those recalling an acceptance or mundane experience (Bernstein, Young, Brown, Sacco, & Claypool, 2008). Accurate iden- tification of non-verbal signals of approach affords an avenue for successful reaffiliation. Because excluded persons have a greater reaffiliative need, it would be essential for them to focus on part- ners most likely to meet these needs, which may be facilitated by accurate perception of real and deceptive smiles. Though this previous work showed that rejected individuals have an acute ability to differentiate between Duchenne and Introduction Being included in groups is essent social relationships feels highly av 1995). Inclusion is so important t evolved mechanisms capable of dete to facilitate reconnection with others Downs, 1995). For example, indivi 0022-1031/$ - see front matter Published by Elsevier doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.08.010 failing to secure stable (Baumeister & Leary, mans appear to have eficits in belongingness eary, Tambor, Terdal, & dispositionally high in engaging in pro-social behaviors than when not (Mehu, Grammer, & Dunbar, 2007). Thus, ‘‘real” smilers are good candidates for poten- tial affiliation whereas fake smilers are potentially deceptive. Given the hazards facing the socially rejected, it may be useful for such persons to quickly and accurately discriminate between facial expressions of emotion (e.g., happy versus sad), and to accu- rately distinguish between real and false signals of motivational in- tent, especially affiliation intentions. Consistent with this logic, we Face perception Emotions 2007) and fully mediated by threats to self-esteem. These results suggest that exclusion yields adaptive FlashReport A preference for genuine smiles followin Michael J. Bernstein a,*, Donald F. Sacco a, Christina M aMiami University, Oxford, OH, United States b St. Louis University, St. Louis, MO, United States a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 3 June 2009 Revised 29 July 2009 Available online 28 August 2009 Keywords: Social exclusion Ostracism Smiles a b s t r a c t Research indicates that re (Duchenne) and deceptive nity). We hypothesized tha uals displaying Duchenne experiences of inclusion, e individuals (10 with Duch much they would like to w participants, excluded indi Journal of Experimen journal homepage: www Inc. social exclusion Brown b, Steven G. Young a, Heather M. Claypool a ed individuals are better than others at discriminating between genuine n-Duchenne) smiles (i.e., true versus false signals of affiliative opportu- jected individuals would show a greater preference to work with individ- sus non-Duchenne smiles. To test this, participants wrote essays about sion, or mundane events. They then saw a series of 20 videos of smiling e and 10 with non-Duchenne smiles). Participants then indicated how with each target. Analyses revealed that compared to included and control als showed a greater preference to work with individuals displaying ‘‘real” le at ScienceDirect al Social Psychology lsevier .com/locate / jesp will attend to and act on these differences in smile types spontane- ously, without having their sincerity explicitly questioned. Participants and design with success (e.g., Maner, DeWall, Baumeister, & Schaller, 2007). Participants then responded to 16 items assessing their levels of belonging, control, self-esteem, and meaningful existence (four items each) felt during the experience (adapted from Zadro, Wil- liams, & Richardson, 2004). Once completed, participants were told they would see videos of individuals and that they were to imagine that the person in each was a potential partner for a project on which they might work. Participants were to indicate how much they would like to work with each person on a Likert-scale (1 = not at all; 7 = very much) for all 20 videos. Upon completion, participants responded to demographic questions, were probed for suspicion, thanked, was a main effect of smile. Participants preferred working with individuals exhibiting Duchenne rather than non-Duchenne enne (M = 4.57, SD = .74) versus non-Duchenne smiles (M = 4.15, 2 M.J. Bernstein et al. / Journal of Experi ental Social Psychology 46 (2010) 196–199 197 One hundred and twenty-five individuals (81 females) partici- pated for course credit and were randomly assigned to a 3 (social experience: exclusion, inclusion, or control) � 2 (smile: Duchenne, non-Duchenne) mixed-model design with repeated measures on the latter. There were no effects of target or participant sex which are not discussed further. Materials The facial stimuli were those used in Bernstein et al. (2008) and were obtained from the BBC science website (http://www.bbc.co. uk/science/humanbody/mind/surveys/smiles).1 Participants watched 20 videos (approximately 4 s each) one at a time, each depicting an individualwith an initially-neutral expression that shifted to a smiling expression, that then returned to a neutral expression (10 Duchenne and 10 non-Duchenne smiles). Thirteen men and seven women were depicted in the videos.2 Presentation orderwas counterbalanced, such that participants saw one of two possible stimuli orders.3 Procedure Participants performed two ostensibly unrelated tasks. They first completed an essay task constituting the manipulation of so- cial experience. Participants wrote about a time they felt ‘‘rejected or excluded,” ‘‘accepted or included,” or ‘‘their morning yesterday” (control condition). This manipulation has been used previously 1 Pre-testing revealed that faces displaying Duchenne versus non-Duchenne smiles did not differ in perceived attractiveness, trustworthiness, or positivity (p > .37). The current research will investigate these issues. We randomly assigned participants to an exclusion, inclusion, or control condi- tion and asked them to rate their desire to work with targets dis- playing both Duchenne and non-Duchenne smiles. Importantly, we did not draw participants’ attention to the targets’ smiles, nor did the instructions ever suggest that smile sincerity varied. We predicted that all perceivers would show a preference for working with targets exhibiting Duchenne rather than non-Duchenne smiles, but that this pattern would be strongest for rejected individuals. Furthermore, we examined possible mechanisms driving this outcome. Williams (2007) has shown that four basic needs are thwarted following rejection which can be grouped into two cate- gories: ‘‘relational” needs (belonging, self-esteem) and ‘‘efficacy/ existence” needs (control, meaningful existence). He has proposed that individuals engage in reaffiliative (rather than antisocial) reac- tions when ‘‘relational needs” are most impacted. Given that the outcome under investigation is affiliative, these ‘‘relational” as op- posed to ‘‘efficacy” needs may operate as the mediator of the Duch- enne preference. Additionally, work on the sociometer model (Leary et al., 1995) argues that self-esteem drops following perceived inclusion threats and motivates humans to engage in behaviors to re-establish their social ties. From this perspective, self-esteem alone may be the key mediator of our proposed find- ing. This work will investigate which of these need threats mediate differential preferences for working with individuals displaying real versus deceptive smiles. Methods 2 The stimuli included three ethnic minorities. Removing these from the analyses left the results unchanged. Thus, all analyses included all stimuli. 3 There were no counterbalancing effects on any results. m SD = .71; p < .001; g = .17). 3.6 3.8 4 4.2 4.4 4.6 4.8 Exclusion Control Inclusion Social Experience D es ire to W or k w ith T ar ge ts Duchenne Non-Duchenne smiles, F(1, 122) = 24.46, p < .001, g2 = .17. As predicted, this effect was qualified by an interaction between social experience and smile, F(2, 122) = 3.26, p = .04, g2 = .05 (Fig. 1). Participants in the control condition showed a marginal preference for individuals exhibiting Duchenne (M = 4.43, SD = .76) versus non-Duchenne smiles (M = 4.28, SD = .67; p = .096; g2 = .02). Participants in the inclusion condition showed a similar marginal effect, Duchenne (M = 4.60, SD = .88) versus non-Duchenne smiles (M = 4.43, SD = .83; p = .055; g2 = .03). Excluded participants, however, showed a significant and larger preference for workingwith those with Duch- and debriefed. Results Basic needs To examine if the social-experience manipulation was success- ful, we calculated each of the four basic needs (belonging, control, self-esteem, meaningful existence) separately for each participant. In all cases, exclusion led to less basic-need satisfaction compared to control and included participants (ps < .001), while the latter two groups did not differ from one another (ps > .35). Preference scores Of primary interest was whether social exclusion influences the desire to work with targets exhibiting Duchenne and non-Duch- enne smiles. For each participant, we averaged (separately) their preference scores for targets with real smiles and those with fake smiles. These averages were subjected to a 3 (social experience: exclusion, control, inclusion) � 2 (smile: Duchenne, non-Duch- enne) mixed-model ANOVA, with repeated measures on the latter. There was no main effect of social experience (p > .52), but there Fig. 1. The effect of social experience on desire to work with targets exhibiting real and fake smiles (error bars represent the standard error of the mean). To further examine the differences between social-experience p < .001). In both cases, excluded participants had less satisfaction of those needs. Relational needs were further related to prefer- Thus, individuals with Duchenne smiles exhibit an affiliative signal. Because rejected individuals should have the strongest Social Experience 0=Inclusion/Control 1=Exclusion Preference-Difference Score Relational Needs β = -1.00* β=.180 (β= .262*) β= -.079+ Fig. 2. The effect of social experience on desire to work with real- versus fake- Social Experience 0= Inclusion/Control 1= Exclusion Preference-Difference Score Self-Esteem β= -.88* β= .166 (β= .262*) β= -.108* 198 M.J. Bernstein et al. / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46 (2010) 196–199 ence–difference scores (b = �.107, p = .008), but efficacy was not (p > .13) and thus cannot act as a mediator. Thus, as individuals felt less satisfaction of their relational needs, they showed a greater preference to work with Duchenne-smile (versus non-Duchenne- smile) targets. Finally, when regressing social experience and rela- tional needs simultaneously onto preference–difference scores, the effect of relational needs remained marginally significant (b = �.08, p = .068), whereas the effect of social experience became non-sig- nificant, b = .18, p = .10. A Sobel test revealed the drop in the mag- nitude of the relationship between social experience and the preference–difference score was marginally significant, z = 1.71, p = .087 (Fig. 2). conditions, for each participant we subtracted his/her average pref- erence score for fake-smile targets from his/her average preference score for real-smile targets, thus creating a ‘‘preference–difference” score where higher numbers indicated a greater preference to work with real-smile targets. A one-way ANOVA on this was signif- icant, F(2122) = 3.26, p = .04; post hoc analyses revealed that while control (M = .15, SD = .55) and inclusion participants (M = .16, SD = .59) did not differ (p > .85), both significantly differed from re- jected participants (M = .42, SD = .51; ps < .035). These results are consistent with the hypothesis that excluded individuals would show an even greater preference for working with Duchenne ver- sus non-Duchenne targets. Mediational analyses As mentioned earlier, Williams (2007) has shown that rejection thwarts four psychological needs. These needs can be divided into two categories which are purported to account for different behav- ioral reactions to rejection. When belonging and self-esteem are most impacted, individuals tend to engage in reaffiliative behav- iors, whereas threats to control and meaningful existence lead to antisocial responses (Williams, 2007). Because our outcome vari- able related to reaffiliation, we suspected that threats to the belonging/self-esteem needs, and not the control/meaningful exis- tence needs, might act as a mediator of our primary effect. We chose to begin our examination by looking at these subcategories of the basic needs as possible mediators. Belonging and self-esteem needs were highly correlated (r = .79, p < .001), as were control and meaningful existence (r = .75, p < .001). We thus averaged belonging and self-esteem needs into one index (Relational Needs) and did the same with control and meaningful existence (Efficacy Needs). In all cases, higher values indicate more satisfaction of the need state.4 Given that the preference scores and basic needs did not differ between inclusion and control conditions, we combined these con- ditions and dummy coded the social-experience variable (0 = inclusion/control, 1 = exclusion). We then used the ‘‘prefer- ence–difference” score as our dependent measure. Following Baron and Kenny (1986), we first examined the rela- tion between social experience (dummy coded) and the prefer- ence–difference score. This relation was significant, b = .26, p = .012. Excluded individuals showed a greater preference for working with Duchenne-smile targets than those in the control/ inclusion conditions. We then found significant relationships be- tween social experience and both possible mediators, threats to relational (b = �1.00, p < .001) and efficacy needs (b = �1.04, 4 The indices of the four basic needs were all significantly correlated with each other (rs > .63, ps < .001), and the relational and efficacy indices were as well (r = .82, p < .001). Given the marginal findings above, we examined belonging and self-esteem separately as possible mediators to compare William’s (2007) and Leary and colleagues’ (1995) models. Both belonging (b = �1.13, p < .001) and self-esteem (b = �.88, p < .001) were pre- dicted by social experience, such that excluded individuals experi- enced less of each than those in the inclusion/control conditions. Further, both belonging (b = �.07, p = .05) and self-esteem (b = �.132, p = .002) predicted the preference–difference score, such that as the participants’ satisfaction of these needs decreased, they showed a greater preference to work with people exhibiting Duchenne versus non-Duchenne smiles. When regressing social experience and belonging simultaneously onto the preference–dif- ference scores the effect of social experience remained significant, (b = .22, p = .05) whereas belonging did not (p > .25). Thus satisfac- tion of belonging needs was not a mediator. However, when regressing social experience and self-esteem simultaneously onto the preference–difference scores, full mediation occurred (Fig. 3); self-esteem remained significant (b = �.11, p = .01) whereas the ef- fect of social experience dropped to below significance (b = .17, p = .12). A Sobel test revealed that this drop in the magnitude of the relationship between social experience and our preference–dif- ference score was significant, z = 2.16, p = .03. Such results are quite consistent with Leary and colleagues’ (1995) sociometer hypothesis. Discussion Bernstein and colleagues (2008) showed that rejected individu- als better discriminate real from deceptive smiles when directly asked to do so. The current work extends this finding by showing that excluded individuals show a greater desire to work with tar- gets exhibiting real versus fake smiles, compared to included or control participants. Duchenne smiles function to convey true feel- ings of positive affect and signal a desire to cooperate while non- Duchenne smiles function only to mask some unknown intent. smiling targets as mediated by relational needs satisfaction (� indicates significant at <05; + indicates marginal significance). Fig. 3. The effect of social experience on desire to work with real- versus fake- smiling targets as mediated by self-esteem needs satisfaction (� indicates signif- icant at <05; + indicates marginal significance). reaffiliative need, we predicted that they should show the greatest desire to work with such targets, and our findings supported this hypothesis. Importantly, participants were not told that smile sin- cerity varied across targets. Thus, without drawing participants’ attention to smile veracity, we illustrated that perceivers, espe- cially excluded ones, spontaneously made judgments favoring Duchenne-smile targets. This work also found that the primary finding was marginally mediated by perceivers’ relational needs satisfaction (the combina- tion of belonging and self-esteem needs). This meditational model showed that exclusion decreased one’s sense of relational needs, and that the more these needs were threatened, the bigger (mar- ginally) one’s preference for Duchenne-smile targets. Interestingly, efficacy needs showed no relationship with the preference mea- sure. Examining these findings vis-à-vis Williams’ (2007) model, which suggests that threats to relational needs are good predictors of pro-social and affiliative responses whereas threats to efficacy needs result in more antisocial and self-focused responses, reveals that the current evidence is congruent with his theorizing. However, the fact that self-esteem alone fully mediated the ef- fect warrants further discussion. Self-esteem is the
/
本文档为【被拒绝者更容易辨别真假微笑】,请使用软件OFFICE或WPS软件打开。作品中的文字与图均可以修改和编辑, 图片更改请在作品中右键图片并更换,文字修改请直接点击文字进行修改,也可以新增和删除文档中的内容。
[版权声明] 本站所有资料为用户分享产生,若发现您的权利被侵害,请联系客服邮件isharekefu@iask.cn,我们尽快处理。 本作品所展示的图片、画像、字体、音乐的版权可能需版权方额外授权,请谨慎使用。 网站提供的党政主题相关内容(国旗、国徽、党徽..)目的在于配合国家政策宣传,仅限个人学习分享使用,禁止用于任何广告和商用目的。

历史搜索

    清空历史搜索